Blog Links:
Understanding Self-Regulation: Starting Points
How Does Self-Regulation Develop?
The Importance of Agency for Self Regulation
Understanding Self-Regulation: Starting Points
How Does Self-Regulation Develop?
The Importance of Agency for Self Regulation
The Importance of Agency for Self-Regulation
One of the key factors in the development of self-regulation skills is the way in which we build our sense of self. In order to learn how to self-regulate, we need to believe that we can determine our own pathways through life, take responsibility for our own choices and decisions, set our own goals and work to achieve them, even when that is hard. We need to believe that it is worth us putting in the effort to do these things because it will improve our experiences of our lives. This gradually developing sense of ourselves as active agents supports us in being motivated to control our impulses, manage our reactions and to handle the challenges we face. Central to the notion of 'self' is the understanding that we have autonomy - that we are active participants in our own lives, working towards goals that we can set for ourselves. When we learn how the choices we make impact on our experiences of our world, we become more motivated to control our behaviours and our emotions.
As children develop, there is an important balance to be achieved between enough agency, and too much. Adults need to manage the levels of risk, creating suitable environments, in which children can experience challenges, aim to achieve goals, and learn how to cope when things do not go to plan. It can be very tempting for adults to over help children - to hover over them, saying 'just let me do that for you' or 'let me show you how that's done' or 'be careful, don't do that!'. (What I tend to call 'helicopter' parenting or teaching.) But where we over protect and over help, children do not learn to believe in themselves, nor do they learn how to cope with challenge and failure, because they do not experience it often enough. What children need is a gradual move from co-regulation with trusted adults, to self-regulation by and for themselves.
Too much scaffolding can lead to a sense of learned helplessness, where the idea that adults will take over and solve everything for you means that you take on less responsibility for yourself. Too much protection can mean that, when we do fail, we struggle to cope with the emotional fallout by ourselves. For agency and self-regulation to develop, children need opportunities to be responsible, to take managed risks and to work towards goals of their own choosing. We need to be willing to do that hardest of things - to sometimes allow our children to fail and to learn by making their own mistakes for themselves. In an educational context, this can be complex to do, because our role as teachers or practitioners is to support learning, and there is often a sense that we need to be 'seen' to be doing something. Standing back or standing aside can feel like a leap of faith when 'just tell them' is the commonly heard cry.
Of course, when we consider the development of self-regulation skills in the context of school, questions about agency and autonomy are tricky to manage. In a class of thirty students, it is not going to be possible for everyone to have agency simultaneously; in a class preparing for an important exam, autonomy may need to be put to one side. In the early years, when many of these key self-regulation skills are starting to develop, and before children begin their compulsory education, there is a strong focus on agency. The high ratios, the focus in the curriculum on developing and caring for the 'whole child', and the use of play as a key part of pedagogy all support the notion of choice, autonomy and personal goal setting.
As the more formal school years begin, and there is an academic curriculum to 'get through' and statutory tests to take, allowing children to have agency can feel risky and too time consuming to manage. There is sometimes an underlying concern that, given the choice, children and young people would choose to take the easy path, or to do nothing. In reality, though, agency is at the heart of intrinsic motivation and is a key aspect in helping children and young people become lifelong learners. All the research into motivation shows that, where we conceptualise learning to be intrinsically valuable to us, we are more likely to engage in it of our own accord. But when the pressure is on teachers to 'get results', allowing young people to learn how to be intrinsically motivated can feel risky or even potentially damaging. Extrinsic motivators can become a very tempting 'go to' alternative.
In a fascinating study, researchers looked at how babies begin to act with purpose (Sloan et al, 2023). They looked at how babies reacted when their feet was tied to a mobile, meaning that they could consciously move it with their feet. A positive feedback loop very quickly developed, whereby the more the mobile moved, the more the baby was incentivised to move, thus producing more movement in the mobile itself. The babies appeared to already understand that there was a cause and effect between themselves and what happened with the mobile. They had effectively set themselves a goal and made it happen. The researchers described the 'aha!' moment that they observed, when the babies realised that it was they themselves who were causing the effect, and rapidly sped up their movements. Even at this very earliest age, human beings are driven to take decisions which allow them to have a direct influence on their experience of their lives.
As I note in my new book, while there is plenty of research into teacher agency, there is relatively little research into children's agency in educational contexts. It is interesting to consider why this is, and to look at different models of education where young people might experience increased levels of agency, for instance in some self-directed learning and home-based education contexts. Agency has been described as the "power to originate action" and is often also described using the term "self-efficacy". Where our aim is for our learners to develop self-regulation, particularly around goal setting and resilience, we should not under estimate the importance of agency and autonomy in building self-efficacy skills.
One simple way to incorporate agency into your classroom is to offer some simple choices. These can often usefully be incorporated during more routine parts of the day. For instance, when self-registering in the mornings, we ask our children to choose whether they would prefer milk or water at snack time, and to put their names on the relevant part of the board. These simple choices can help children feel like they do have an element of agency and influence, within their classroom setting. Similarly, saying to a child who is hesitant to start writing, "would you prefer to start by doing this, or by doing that?", offers a straightforward way to add an element of agency to learning in class.
Reference:
Sloan, Jones and Scott Kelso, Meaning from Movement and Stillness: signatures of coordination dynamics reveal infant agency, 2023 (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences)
Find out more about my new book, or purchase a copy by clicking on the image below. I also offer online and in-person CPD/training sessions on Supporting the Development of Self-Regulation. Please email me for more details.
One of the key factors in the development of self-regulation skills is the way in which we build our sense of self. In order to learn how to self-regulate, we need to believe that we can determine our own pathways through life, take responsibility for our own choices and decisions, set our own goals and work to achieve them, even when that is hard. We need to believe that it is worth us putting in the effort to do these things because it will improve our experiences of our lives. This gradually developing sense of ourselves as active agents supports us in being motivated to control our impulses, manage our reactions and to handle the challenges we face. Central to the notion of 'self' is the understanding that we have autonomy - that we are active participants in our own lives, working towards goals that we can set for ourselves. When we learn how the choices we make impact on our experiences of our world, we become more motivated to control our behaviours and our emotions.
As children develop, there is an important balance to be achieved between enough agency, and too much. Adults need to manage the levels of risk, creating suitable environments, in which children can experience challenges, aim to achieve goals, and learn how to cope when things do not go to plan. It can be very tempting for adults to over help children - to hover over them, saying 'just let me do that for you' or 'let me show you how that's done' or 'be careful, don't do that!'. (What I tend to call 'helicopter' parenting or teaching.) But where we over protect and over help, children do not learn to believe in themselves, nor do they learn how to cope with challenge and failure, because they do not experience it often enough. What children need is a gradual move from co-regulation with trusted adults, to self-regulation by and for themselves.
Too much scaffolding can lead to a sense of learned helplessness, where the idea that adults will take over and solve everything for you means that you take on less responsibility for yourself. Too much protection can mean that, when we do fail, we struggle to cope with the emotional fallout by ourselves. For agency and self-regulation to develop, children need opportunities to be responsible, to take managed risks and to work towards goals of their own choosing. We need to be willing to do that hardest of things - to sometimes allow our children to fail and to learn by making their own mistakes for themselves. In an educational context, this can be complex to do, because our role as teachers or practitioners is to support learning, and there is often a sense that we need to be 'seen' to be doing something. Standing back or standing aside can feel like a leap of faith when 'just tell them' is the commonly heard cry.
Of course, when we consider the development of self-regulation skills in the context of school, questions about agency and autonomy are tricky to manage. In a class of thirty students, it is not going to be possible for everyone to have agency simultaneously; in a class preparing for an important exam, autonomy may need to be put to one side. In the early years, when many of these key self-regulation skills are starting to develop, and before children begin their compulsory education, there is a strong focus on agency. The high ratios, the focus in the curriculum on developing and caring for the 'whole child', and the use of play as a key part of pedagogy all support the notion of choice, autonomy and personal goal setting.
As the more formal school years begin, and there is an academic curriculum to 'get through' and statutory tests to take, allowing children to have agency can feel risky and too time consuming to manage. There is sometimes an underlying concern that, given the choice, children and young people would choose to take the easy path, or to do nothing. In reality, though, agency is at the heart of intrinsic motivation and is a key aspect in helping children and young people become lifelong learners. All the research into motivation shows that, where we conceptualise learning to be intrinsically valuable to us, we are more likely to engage in it of our own accord. But when the pressure is on teachers to 'get results', allowing young people to learn how to be intrinsically motivated can feel risky or even potentially damaging. Extrinsic motivators can become a very tempting 'go to' alternative.
In a fascinating study, researchers looked at how babies begin to act with purpose (Sloan et al, 2023). They looked at how babies reacted when their feet was tied to a mobile, meaning that they could consciously move it with their feet. A positive feedback loop very quickly developed, whereby the more the mobile moved, the more the baby was incentivised to move, thus producing more movement in the mobile itself. The babies appeared to already understand that there was a cause and effect between themselves and what happened with the mobile. They had effectively set themselves a goal and made it happen. The researchers described the 'aha!' moment that they observed, when the babies realised that it was they themselves who were causing the effect, and rapidly sped up their movements. Even at this very earliest age, human beings are driven to take decisions which allow them to have a direct influence on their experience of their lives.
As I note in my new book, while there is plenty of research into teacher agency, there is relatively little research into children's agency in educational contexts. It is interesting to consider why this is, and to look at different models of education where young people might experience increased levels of agency, for instance in some self-directed learning and home-based education contexts. Agency has been described as the "power to originate action" and is often also described using the term "self-efficacy". Where our aim is for our learners to develop self-regulation, particularly around goal setting and resilience, we should not under estimate the importance of agency and autonomy in building self-efficacy skills.
One simple way to incorporate agency into your classroom is to offer some simple choices. These can often usefully be incorporated during more routine parts of the day. For instance, when self-registering in the mornings, we ask our children to choose whether they would prefer milk or water at snack time, and to put their names on the relevant part of the board. These simple choices can help children feel like they do have an element of agency and influence, within their classroom setting. Similarly, saying to a child who is hesitant to start writing, "would you prefer to start by doing this, or by doing that?", offers a straightforward way to add an element of agency to learning in class.
Reference:
Sloan, Jones and Scott Kelso, Meaning from Movement and Stillness: signatures of coordination dynamics reveal infant agency, 2023 (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences)
Find out more about my new book, or purchase a copy by clicking on the image below. I also offer online and in-person CPD/training sessions on Supporting the Development of Self-Regulation. Please email me for more details.
How Does Self-Regulation Develop?
When thinking about how self-regulation develops, it's helpful to think about the changes that happen during early childhood in evolutionary terms. What we think of as important 'executive functions' for living in modern society have developed over time to help us cope and thrive in an increasingly busy and stressful world, where society puts specific requirements on us around our behaviours. For instance, in a classroom situation, it can feel problematic when children become distracted: by a wasp, by a worker digging up the road outside your room, or by a sudden flurry of snow. But, in reality, the very act of turning towards 'a distraction' is an evolutionary adaptation designed to keep us safe. When the 'loud noise' could have been a wild animal, prowling outside the cave where you are sheltering, it is very important to be drawn towards that noise. Children only gradually learn to inhibit these kinds of instinctive responses, in conjunction with the modelling and support of adults, and learning alongside their peers.
Babies are not born able to self-regulate - in fact, it would be problematic if they were, because self-regulation develops in partnership with language and the stages we go through in learning to self-regulate are crucial in terms of our development. Babies need to cry, in order to communicate to us that there is some kind of problem - perhaps they are tired, hungry, stressed, in pain. Babies need to be what we might call 'distractible' - they are drawn to explore the world around them, turning or reaching towards whatever draws their attention, and this again is how they learn and develop. It's also useful to understand that, when babies are born, their senses are not fully developed, particularly their eyesight. They can only very gradually start to 'make sense' of the fuzzy world around them, through interacting with it and physically experiencing it. Gradually, as babies and young children learn how to use language to express themselves and ask questions, they start to learn how to 'think through' their actions, inhibit some responses, and develop social behaviours and social communication. It is no coincidence that difficulty with self-regulation is often seen alongside under-developed language skills.
The experiences that we go through in early childhood are a key part of how we develop self-regulation skills, with around 90 percent of brain development taking place in those crucial first five years. There are genetic and epigenetic elements in the development of self-regulation, but nurture, environment and experiences play a critical part. In order for babies to develop self-regulation skills, what they most need is sensitive, loving adult caregivers who interact warmly with them and reliably meet their needs. Adults must use we refer to as 'co-regulation', or working together with the child to support them to learn how to self-regulate. Carers need to show what we call in a classroom context 'unconditional positive regard' - that their care and love is not predicated on the behaviour of the child and will not be withdrawn unexpectedly. This in turn allows young children to feel safe, secure and able to explore and take risks, because even if they get something wrong or struggle, they will still be soothed, comforted and loved, as a person in their own right. Alongside these approaches, carers also use what are referred to as 'serve and return' interactions with the child. This term describes the crucial shared interactions between a parent or carer and their child involving gesture, facial expressions and language, which are so powerful in building language, communication and connections in the child's rapidly developing brain.
Not only do all these interactions help build language skills, but crucially they allow babies and young children to feel safe in exploring, giving things a go, making mistakes and learning from the results. The adult caregivers create an environment where it is safe to explore, managing risk on behalf of the young child. Where the child finds things too difficult and becomes distressed, the caregivers comfort the child and demonstrate that it is okay to try and struggle. Interestingly, too much interference from adults can be as problematic as too little - we have to support young children to take managed risks for themselves, at the appropriate moment, in order for them to learn and grow. A great example of this is knowing when to let a child pour their own drink - it is likely at first that they will spill the drink. Where adults over react to this 'mistake', the child might draw back from trying again; where adults over help and never let the child have a go at pouring the drink, similarly they will never learn to do so by themselves. The role of agency - of letting the child 'have a go' at things they are interested in doing, is fundamental to the development of self-regulation. As I often find myself saying, there is a reason why the word 'self' is part of the term. We cannot do it for them, and indeed we should not want to, because our goal is to build independence and self-esteem. Children need to learn to see themselves as active agents, shaping their own experiences of their world, in order to fully develop their self-regulation skills.
Another interesting aspect in the development of self-regulation is the reliable availability of resources. Again, thinking about self-regulation in evolutionary terms, this is a key factor in what allowed humans to become social animals and to work in groups. Where resources are scarce, we might see behaviours that we label as 'dysregulated' - snatching, grabbing, stealing. As human beings learned how to regulate, resources could more reliably be shared around a group of people, with social communication being used to decide how this might take place (for instance, through bartering). In a classroom context, if you think about a child who has been brought up with very little access to toys, or perhaps in a situation where there is food poverty, the instinctive reaction might be to grab or snatch from another child, for fear of that scarcity meaning they do not get a look in. When people say that 'behaviour is communication', this is a great example of how an inappropriate behaviour (snatching) can indicate an underlying issue that is about the child's early experiences and development, rather than what some commentators might label 'bad' behaviour.
Finally, it is very useful to remember that these skills develop over time, and that they do not all develop simultaneously. Research has shown that empathy is one of the later developing skills. We actually see the early signs of empathy in very young babies - where they will start to cry in response to the sound of another baby in distress. But it is only once they have developed 'theory of mind' that they can start to understand how to fully 'place' themselves in the position of another. I'll return to the fascinating subject of empathy, and share some thoughts about building this skill in a classroom context, in the next in this series of blogs. My latest book, which looks at these topics in much greater depth, is The Ultimate Guide to Self-Regulation, published in January 2025. You can pre-order a copy here.
When thinking about how self-regulation develops, it's helpful to think about the changes that happen during early childhood in evolutionary terms. What we think of as important 'executive functions' for living in modern society have developed over time to help us cope and thrive in an increasingly busy and stressful world, where society puts specific requirements on us around our behaviours. For instance, in a classroom situation, it can feel problematic when children become distracted: by a wasp, by a worker digging up the road outside your room, or by a sudden flurry of snow. But, in reality, the very act of turning towards 'a distraction' is an evolutionary adaptation designed to keep us safe. When the 'loud noise' could have been a wild animal, prowling outside the cave where you are sheltering, it is very important to be drawn towards that noise. Children only gradually learn to inhibit these kinds of instinctive responses, in conjunction with the modelling and support of adults, and learning alongside their peers.
Babies are not born able to self-regulate - in fact, it would be problematic if they were, because self-regulation develops in partnership with language and the stages we go through in learning to self-regulate are crucial in terms of our development. Babies need to cry, in order to communicate to us that there is some kind of problem - perhaps they are tired, hungry, stressed, in pain. Babies need to be what we might call 'distractible' - they are drawn to explore the world around them, turning or reaching towards whatever draws their attention, and this again is how they learn and develop. It's also useful to understand that, when babies are born, their senses are not fully developed, particularly their eyesight. They can only very gradually start to 'make sense' of the fuzzy world around them, through interacting with it and physically experiencing it. Gradually, as babies and young children learn how to use language to express themselves and ask questions, they start to learn how to 'think through' their actions, inhibit some responses, and develop social behaviours and social communication. It is no coincidence that difficulty with self-regulation is often seen alongside under-developed language skills.
The experiences that we go through in early childhood are a key part of how we develop self-regulation skills, with around 90 percent of brain development taking place in those crucial first five years. There are genetic and epigenetic elements in the development of self-regulation, but nurture, environment and experiences play a critical part. In order for babies to develop self-regulation skills, what they most need is sensitive, loving adult caregivers who interact warmly with them and reliably meet their needs. Adults must use we refer to as 'co-regulation', or working together with the child to support them to learn how to self-regulate. Carers need to show what we call in a classroom context 'unconditional positive regard' - that their care and love is not predicated on the behaviour of the child and will not be withdrawn unexpectedly. This in turn allows young children to feel safe, secure and able to explore and take risks, because even if they get something wrong or struggle, they will still be soothed, comforted and loved, as a person in their own right. Alongside these approaches, carers also use what are referred to as 'serve and return' interactions with the child. This term describes the crucial shared interactions between a parent or carer and their child involving gesture, facial expressions and language, which are so powerful in building language, communication and connections in the child's rapidly developing brain.
Not only do all these interactions help build language skills, but crucially they allow babies and young children to feel safe in exploring, giving things a go, making mistakes and learning from the results. The adult caregivers create an environment where it is safe to explore, managing risk on behalf of the young child. Where the child finds things too difficult and becomes distressed, the caregivers comfort the child and demonstrate that it is okay to try and struggle. Interestingly, too much interference from adults can be as problematic as too little - we have to support young children to take managed risks for themselves, at the appropriate moment, in order for them to learn and grow. A great example of this is knowing when to let a child pour their own drink - it is likely at first that they will spill the drink. Where adults over react to this 'mistake', the child might draw back from trying again; where adults over help and never let the child have a go at pouring the drink, similarly they will never learn to do so by themselves. The role of agency - of letting the child 'have a go' at things they are interested in doing, is fundamental to the development of self-regulation. As I often find myself saying, there is a reason why the word 'self' is part of the term. We cannot do it for them, and indeed we should not want to, because our goal is to build independence and self-esteem. Children need to learn to see themselves as active agents, shaping their own experiences of their world, in order to fully develop their self-regulation skills.
Another interesting aspect in the development of self-regulation is the reliable availability of resources. Again, thinking about self-regulation in evolutionary terms, this is a key factor in what allowed humans to become social animals and to work in groups. Where resources are scarce, we might see behaviours that we label as 'dysregulated' - snatching, grabbing, stealing. As human beings learned how to regulate, resources could more reliably be shared around a group of people, with social communication being used to decide how this might take place (for instance, through bartering). In a classroom context, if you think about a child who has been brought up with very little access to toys, or perhaps in a situation where there is food poverty, the instinctive reaction might be to grab or snatch from another child, for fear of that scarcity meaning they do not get a look in. When people say that 'behaviour is communication', this is a great example of how an inappropriate behaviour (snatching) can indicate an underlying issue that is about the child's early experiences and development, rather than what some commentators might label 'bad' behaviour.
Finally, it is very useful to remember that these skills develop over time, and that they do not all develop simultaneously. Research has shown that empathy is one of the later developing skills. We actually see the early signs of empathy in very young babies - where they will start to cry in response to the sound of another baby in distress. But it is only once they have developed 'theory of mind' that they can start to understand how to fully 'place' themselves in the position of another. I'll return to the fascinating subject of empathy, and share some thoughts about building this skill in a classroom context, in the next in this series of blogs. My latest book, which looks at these topics in much greater depth, is The Ultimate Guide to Self-Regulation, published in January 2025. You can pre-order a copy here.
Understanding Self-Regulation: Starting Points
Educators have increasingly taken an interest in the topic of self-regulation over recent years. Not only does self-regulation offer us a new way to think about young people's behaviour, but there is plenty of research linking executive function skills to positive educational and wider outcomes. Anecdotally, since the start of the pandemic, more and more schools and settings are reporting increased levels of needs in learners. There is a desire to find solutions which support young people to develop self-control, regulate their emotions and manage their own behaviours. Rather than only focusing on external systems to do the work of managing behaviour in our settings, educators are increasingly looking to find ways to help learners internalise the skills involved in maintaining appropriate behaviours for learning in class.
In England, the EYFS Statutory Framework includes reference to some key self-regulation skills, grouping them under the heading of "Personal, Social and Emotional Development" (PSED). PSED is one of the 'prime' (or key) areas of early learning. It underpins healthy development and educational progress alongside the two other prime areas of "Communication and Language" and "Physical Development". It is interesting to note the level of expectations in the way that this part of the latest Statutory Framework is phrased. Not only are children expected to develop a positive sense of self, but they must also have confidence in their abilities, persist and wait for what they want, direct their attention "as necessary" and "resolve conflicts peaceably". Observers might note that even adults often struggle with these skills. While they are a key part of development for the 0-5 age range, the idea that they might be fully developed by the end of the early years seems wildly optimistic, particularly at present.
A good way to understand self-regulation skills is to think of them as being like an "air traffic control system" for the brain. This metaphor is explained further in this excellent working paper from Harvard's Center for the Developing Child. Essentially, this is a set of skills which help us to manage ourselves within the range of different environments we might find ourselves in - that metaphorically stop us from 'bumping into' each other in social, educational and other contexts. They allow us to guide our own 'flight paths' through our lives, managing our responses, staying on an even emotional keel and coping with the challenges that we face.
There are three key areas of executive (or brain) function involved in our ability to regulate ourselves: these are working memory, impulse control and mental flexibility. Working memory is important because it can help us think things through in our minds - thinking about our actions is a key facet of self-regulation. It also helps us remember a series of things we might need to do, ticking them off mentally one by one, for instance when we need to follow instructions. Impulse control allows us to stop ourselves from responding immediately to external stimuli, particularly where it might not be helpful or appropriate. Finally, mental flexibility allows us to face challenges and come up with alternative solutions to try and solve problems. This helps us to avoid the situation where we come up against an issue but repeatedly take the same linear path to attempt (and fail) to resolve it.
When thinking about the behaviours which fall under the umbrella term of 'self-regulation', this encompasses a wide-ranging set of skills. While there has been a relatively strong focus in schools on supporting the development of emotional regulation, in fact this area forms only part of the picture. Self-regulation is about becoming aware of, understanding and learning to control our behaviours, including the development of impulse control and deferred gratification. It involves the ability to control our attention, concentrating on what we need to, learning how to manage distractions and move our focus around as needed. It involves becoming aware of our feelings, understanding their source, learning how to handle strong emotions and building the skill of empathy with others. And it also involves goal-oriented behaviours, such as setting ourselves sensible targets, working to achieve these, and coping with both challenge and failure.
Underpinning the development of self-regulation is the vital need for agency: it is crucial to focus in on the 'self' part of the descriptor term 'self-regulation'. If we are going to learn how to manage ourselves, we cannot rely only on habituated responses to external stimuli. We need to learn to focus in on ourselves and our personal experiences of the world, building a sense of self and gradually developing the ability to manage our reactions to the range of situations we encounter. This, I think, might be one of the reasons why we are seeing young people and also educators pushing back against those school systems which exert ever greater control in order to 'manage behaviour'. If we want young people to build intrinsic motivation and a sense of self, they need to believe that they have the power to choose their own behaviours for themselves and, in so doing, to change and improve their experience of their world.
In the next blog in this series, I will look at the various factors which need to come together to support the development of self-regulation. My latest book, which looks at all these topics in much greater depth, is The Ultimate Guide to Self-Regulation, published in January 2025. You can pre-order a copy here.